How We Read

In the U.S., most of us can read. But still very little is known about how our brains actually do it. Up next — a scientist who studies what’s going on in our brains when we read.

How We Read 1

Dr. Guinevere Eden is Director of Georgetown University’s Center for the Study of Learning. She’s in the third year of a five-year study that’s looking at what parts of the brain we use to read.

Guinevere Eden: Unlike the rest of the brain that sometimes has evolved to be very good at different types of tasks, reading is something that we’re not born to do, it’s something that we teach our brain. So we want to know, where in the brain does reading happen.

How We Read 2
“Woman Reading,” Pierre Auguste Renoir.

To find out, Eden and her colleagues use “fMRI” — or functional magnetic resonance imaging. It uses a kind of giant magnet that can detect which areas of the brain are working harder. Volunteers lie inside the magnet reading. Computers translate the magnet’s signal into a visual image.

The researchers have found several areas in the brain that consistently light up when we read. Part of Eden’s current research focuses on finding out what those areas of the brain were evolved to do in the first place.

Guinevere Eden: Why did these areas get chosen and what is it about there areas that makes them good for the reading process and … why do they consistently get picked?

Recent fMRI scans show that people use similar brain areas for reading as they do for processing visual motion — such as identifying the direction of moving dots on a screen.

Eden and her colleagues have found several areas in the brain that consistently light up when we read — but not as brightly in people with the reading disorder dyslexia as in brain images from normal readers.

Excerpts from an interview with Dr. Eden:

We try to understand where in the brain reading occurs. And reading is something that we go to school, we’re explicitly taught how to read, and somewhere in the brain, that process must be taking place.

Can you tell me about your current research?

We use brain imaging technology to try to understand what is the signature for reading in the brain, and how does it differ in children who have a diagnosis of dyslexia

What does “the signature of reading” mean?

We try to understand where in the brain reading occurs. And reading is something that we go to school, we’re explicitly taught how to read, and somewhere in the brain, that process must be taking place.

Unlike the rest of the brain that sometimes has evolved to be very good at different types of tasks, reading is something that we’re not born to do, it’s something that we teach our brain. So we want to know, where in the brain does reading happen.

How does a functional MRI work and how does it allow you to find out what parts of our brain we use when we read?

What the functional MRI does is it tells us is where there’s an increase of blood flow to certain parts of the brain, and we use that as an indication of areas where the brain is working hard and where there is normal activity. And so we can ask people to do a task inside the magnet, for example, reading words, and then see where the brain increases as they’re doing that task

The nice thing about functional magnetic resonance imaging is it’s non-invasive, so we’re using our own bloodstream as our indoginous marker of where there is normal activity

One of the other research interests that we have is to not only understand what are the areas we use to read and how do they differ in people with dyslexia, but how did they get roped into the reading process in the first place. Because we have so many areas in the brain that have become evolved for a specific task…through evolution, they’ve made us better at surviving. We have areas in the brain whose only job in life is to tell us when something is moving in our periphery. And the reason we have that is because if we didn’t, we would have been eaten by someone who would have moved in through the periphery and had us for lunch. Those are the very old areas in the brain that we have maintained. But reading has only been around for four thousand years — and has only been actively practiced for maybe a thousand years, not even. So when we teach someone to learn to read, we’re asking them to something that’s not really a very natural task and that we haven’t evolved to do. And so the question is the areas that do get involved in the reading process, what were they designed to do in the first place, and why is it that they got picked to become involved in reading?

And how do you answer that question?

By asking people to do not only reading tasks, but other tasks that we think may share something with reading, in terms of the mechanisms, even though they may not obviously seem related to reading. One of those tasks actually happens to be perception of moving objects, because we know that people who have dyslexia have very subtle deficits in visual motion discrimination. It’s not clear to us how this relates to their reading deficit, but it may be that areas in the brain that are involved in reading and areas in the brain that are involved in visual motion perception have some property, the neurons may have something special that allows them to become very good at these two different processes. It may have something to do with the timing — we’re not quite sure yet.

It’s twofold research. One is to really characterize dyslexia more carefully and then go on to understand which of these behavioral issues that we see in dyslexia are the ones that are most amenable to intervention and would then bring about better reading gains.

And this is the thing that’s so exciting about this technology — which is really still very young — is that you can actually see differences in a person with dyslexia once they have gone through a successful treatment program and it’s really important to know what those areas are, because it tells us something about how the intervention is working. 12.02 If we know which areas change, we know a little bit about what those parts of the brain do and why maybe they have become the target of the intervention and what it may be about the intervention that we may need to maximize to make it an even more effective intervention

We’re still really just at the beginning…We suspect that different parts of the brain are involved in different aspects of the reading process. People are only just beginning to work those out

Imaging can be very good at helping in some of those, but maybe is harder to use imaging for some other aspects, depending on how well these tasks lend themselves to lying on your back, inside a tunnel. as you’re reading these words and being able to do it in the kind of experimental constraints that we sometimes have with brain imaging.

How has your research changed since the advent of the fMRI?

We can now scan children, which previous imaging methods didn’t allow us to do. And if you want to understand what is the mechanism of dyslexia, you really want to see the brain in young dyslexic children, rather than studying adults, who have already had a lifelong history of reading problems.

Why do there areas get chosen and what is it about there areas that makes them good for the reading process and… why do they consistently get picked? One question is…what if you read in French or German or Spanish?

How did you end up doing what you do?

I came to America to do a fellowship with the National Institute of Health to learn how to do brain imaging. Because there is no animal model of reading. You know, you can study several disorders by developing animal models, but you cannot teach a rat or a monkey to read. And there’s only really one way to understand the reading human brain and that’s by using a non-invasive imaging technology.

What are your research plans for the next few months?

We have a lot of projects that are ongoing. We study typical readers. We study adults and children with dyslexia. One of the questions we want to know is, why kind of interventions are effective in changing people’s ability to learn to read. Are they the same for adults as they are for children? Are they the same for every dyslexic? There probably isn’t one approach that works for every individual who has dyslexia. We may have to learn more about subtypes of dyslexia, see if the subtypes have different brain maps that go with them and also that they might then respond slightly differently to different kinds of interventions.

The hardest thing is making the environment comfortable for children. We scan children as young as six and five years of age….you have to go into it with a frame of mind knowing that you have to be extremely patient you have prepare the child extremely well and you have to not just work with your colleagues and that child, but also with their family…and we want to work with the school as well… 26.58 children like it, because that look on it as a science project, but you have to make it interesting for them

Who is funding your research?

It’s funded through National Institutes of Health — Child Health and Human Development

I’ll tell you what I think is the best thing that’s come out of this research…I think for children to recognize that the problems they’re having aren’t their fault helps them tremendously…that there are studies going on that are trying to work out more about what’s going on inside their heads so we can come up with better ways of helping them.

The fact that there’s an image that shows you that there’s a difference between someone with dyslexia and someone without dyslexia I think has helped people understand that …it’s not about being lazy or having the wrong attitude. It truly is a biological disorder.

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Scroll to Top